Eileen GSell
On Gunning�s
An Aesthetics of Astonishment
Unabashedly
wary of what he deems certain historians surprisingly ahistorical
rendition of cinemas first audiences,
Gunning takes pains to detail just how myopic (if subtly so) certain
contemporary theorizations of spectatorship prove to be. The terrorized spectator so often
mythologized by certain film traditions is not the naïve, unfettered viewer as
many claim. Instead, Gunning insists, early audiences actively participated in
their own suspension of belief, as part of a longstanding era in which
the photographic or filmic apparatus itself stuns as much as, if not more than,
the unbelievable nature of illusion. The
first modes of exhibition emphasized the technological accomplishment of moving
pictures, as well as the films place within an aesthetic tradition of illusions
that included trompe loeil,
freak shows, and a general obsession with the unnatural. Early Lumiere films were initially presented as frozen images so
as to draw attention to the movement of the projector, precluding any
experience of the film as reality.
Presented as a succession of attractions, the earliest movies were often
shown alongside live performances, musical numbers, and any number of other
acts, within a smorgasboard of sensual thrills, a
discontinuous series of diversions. The
resultant disbelief of the audience was then an act of will, not earnest,
passive credulity (what we often relate to getting lost in cinemas narrative
strains, which back then barely existed).
Though far
from quaintly gullible, early film viewers were likely more vulnerable to the
addictive qualities of aesthetic attractions than they themselves
realized. Pleasure is the issue here,
Gunning asserts, even if pleasure of a particularly complicated sort. While the allure of cheap thrills goes all
the way back to Augustines fifth century curiositas, within modernity the enormous popularity of these spectacles
results from an all-encompassing feeling of lack. Attractions are a response
to an experience of alienation, the loss of fulfilling experience, the
constant onslaught of distraction.
Bringing in Gorky, Kracauer, Benjamin, and
good old Shivelbusch, Gunning describes earliest film
viewers as people who needed the shock of cinema, not only for scopic pleasure, but to make sense of a world in which
spatial and temporal relations were radically transforming.
Even as I
accept this premise, I somewhat question whether such a dynamic has survived in
similar forms today. If it does, this
would seem to suggest that even when enjoying the most narrative movies, we do so discontinuously, allowing ourselves to
become absorbed only intermittently. In Bergsonian
terms, we operate even closer to the tip of the cone, so removed from pure
memory that ideas of a Metzian inner credulous
viewer seem almost risible. How might our responses to the films on Monday
support such a theory? How might they refute it?